Hopefully some interesting bits of information extracted from science and non-fiction literature. (For historical reasons there are some poems scattered throughout this blog.) Sachthemen und Sachtexte. (Historisch finden sich auf diesem Blog auch einige Gedichte und Aphorismen.) [Just collecting some exciting bits of information here.] [Eine Sammlung von interessanten Texten, Fragen und Antworten will das hier sein. Nicht mehr, aber auch nicht weniger.]
Mittwoch, 28. Dezember 2016
Sonntag, 25. Dezember 2016
Samstag, 3. Dezember 2016
Sonntag, 27. November 2016
Samstag, 26. November 2016
Richard D. Alexander on runaway selection:
Richard D. Alexander (2005)
"Ronald A. Fisher (1930) used the adjective runaway to apply to a hypothetical
selective process that he assigned to sexual selection (see also Trivers 1972; Miller
2000). I suggest that sexual selection and reciprocity selection are special forms of
the broader concept of social selection. Social selection is a consequence of competition
among individuals for rewards arising out of various kinds of social or mutualistic
beneficence.
Fisher’s runaway sexual selection has two special features. First is the tendency
of the choosing parties to begin favoring individuals with traits that are extreme
on some axis of desirability, rather than favoring some particular condition (other
than extremeness) of the individuals to be chosen. This form of choosing can
only occur in an organism that is able to compare an array of individuals and identify
desirable extremes (e.g., Alexander, Marshall, and Cooley 1997). It can evolve
to become the standard method of choice only if selection continues for a long time
in one direction and the best choices continue to be beyond the previous expressions
of the trait. Thus, if females that choose extreme males outreproduce those
that do not, the choosing of extremes will spread. So will extremeness in males;
otherwise the females choosing them would not gain. Spreading or fixing the choosing
of extremes will inject a certain degree of inertia into the process. For this reason,
once an “extreme-choosing” tendency is in place, extremes in the traits of the
chosen individuals can pass beyond the form in which they are adaptive in any other
sense and indeed can become disadvantageous in other contexts. Some such conflict
actually exists among all the compromises of selection on different traits of the organism
because the effect of selection can only be enhancement of the reproductive
integrity of the organism as a whole rather than the state of any of its individual
traits. When selection is social, however—when it is a matter of individuals choosing
other individuals in a mutualistic or reciprocal social interaction rather than, say,
competition to detect or capture food, or to escape enemies more effectively—overshoots in adaptiveness in other respects, because of choosing extremeness,
will be more prominent.
The second special feature of Fisher’s runaway sexual selection is the feedback
resulting from the genetic partnership between males and females in jointly
produced offspring. Trivers (1972, 166) described it as follows:
>[I]f there is a tendency for females to sample the male distribution and to prefer
one extreme (for example, the more brightly colored males), then selection
will move the male distribution toward the favoured extreme. After a one
generation lag, the distribution of female preferences will also move toward a
greater percentage of females with extreme desires, because the granddaughters
of females preferring the favoured extreme will be more numerous than
the granddaughters of females favoring other male attributes. Until countervailing
selection intervenes, this female preference will, as first pointed out
by Fisher (1958), move both male attributes and female preferences with increasing
rapidity in the same direction.<
We can note, first, that some aspects of human evolution that intrigue us, such
as brain functions that result in cleverness in social interactions, including scenario
building and testing the social future by weighing alternatives internally, could
have evolved partly through sexual selection. In view of the tendency of the human
brain to become larger across history, and of human behavior to become more
complex—and seemingly ever more rapidly after these features had exceeded their
counterparts in other species—we might be concerned to examine the likelihood
that runaway sexual selection is involved. A related question is whether some or all
features of runaway selection can also occur in nonsexual social interactions such
as the high-risk forms of social reciprocity that are prominent only in the human
species.
At first one may imagine that there are no parallels in social selection allowing
it to become runaway. But the way an individual gains by selecting its social
partners parallels the ways an individual can gain from cooperative interactions
with a mate and from a mate’s parental care. In both cases there is likely to be genetic
change in both the ability to choose good partners and the background of
the favored phenotypic attributes because a mutually beneficial interaction can be
maintained only if, on average, both interactants gain.
Sexual selection is a distinctive kind of runaway selection because joint production
of offspring by the interacting pair causes the process to accelerate (see
above Trivers quote). The defining feature of runaway selection is not acceleration,
however, but the tendency of the process to go significantly beyond adaptiveness
in all contexts except within the particular selective race—much further beyond
adaptiveness in other contexts than is ordinarily the case in the myriad compromises
among the conflicting adaptive traits that create and maintain the unified organism.
This aspect of runaway selection may hold for reciprocity selection, in which,
unlike in sexual selection, both parties can carry tendencies not only to choose extremes but also to display extremes. In social selection, again unlike in sexual selection
(except in simultaneous hermaphrodites), an individual can play both roles,
of chooser and chosen, with respect to the same traits, and alternations of roles
can occur during extended interactions between particular partners. To the extent
that social success in ecologically dominant humans (see earlier) becomes the
central determinant of reproductive success, runaway reciprocity selection may
be a more viable possibility than Fisherian runaway sexual selection.
Fisherian runaway sexual selection presumably begins with a likelihood of
heritability (i.e., genetic variability) in both variations in choices and variations in
chosen traits. It is easy to see that in this circumstance such competitions, or
races, will lead to genetic changes, at first changing both the ability and tendency
to choose extremes and the nature of the extremes available for choice. Continuing
mutations and genetic recombination (outbreeding among temporarily isolated
groups) will tend to offer the choosing parties increasingly extreme possible
choices, though to a reduced degree as selection continues to remove heritable
variations in trait expressions and the trait becomes sufficiently maladaptive in contexts
other than sexual selection. From earlier arguments it is not easy to understand
the extent to which relevant heritability is likely to disappear, or even become
trivial.
Diminution (or disappearance) of heritability in variations will not necessarily
change the tendency to choose extremes: in effect, if an ability and tendency to
choose extremes in any of a variety of social races (at least in humans) could become
genetically fixed in the population (including the ability and tendency to learn
from others, or from observation, the values of such choosing), it could still offer
advantages to the chooser of extremes (without evolutionary change in the trait
per se) because of the usefulness of even nonheritable trait variations chosen in a
social partner. Of course, there may be further evolutionary improvements in the
ability to identify and use extreme traits even after all choosers are already choosing
extremes.
On the other hand, heritable variations in what is chosen will result in a continued
march toward greater extremes because this relative quality will not be fixed
in the way the ability to identify and favor extremes can be fixed; extremes can be
identified only by comparing whatever is available. In Fisher’s version of runaway selection,
extremes win reproductively at first because they are ecologically superior,
meaning functionally superior outside the choice situation itself. Later they continue
to win because they are sexually (or, here, socially) superior, even though, as
a result of the progress of selection involving choice of extremes, they may have be come so extreme as to be otherwise functionally (ecologically) inferior. Here, “sexually
or socially superior” means that the choosing parties will have acquired a genetic
composition that will cause them to choose the extreme, the resulting choice
of the extreme individuals itself causing the chosen individuals to outreproduce.
Extremes, however, will be chosen whether or not, as extremes, they represent
heritable variants. If extreme social performances yield special social opportunities
to those displaying them (e.g., via their reputations as achievers or winners),
then regardless of the basis for the superior performances (i.e., genetic variant or
not), winning performances can yield benefits to the kin or other social associates
of the individual with the extreme traits. Thus merely joining social competitions
or races can pay, though only heritable variations, including the (variant) ability to
choose the best from among multiple available races that might be entered, will
yield evolutionary change. Heritable variations in the ability to choose appropriate
races—including not only those generally likely to be profitable but those in which
the choosing individual, because of his or her special traits, has a special likelihood
of competing successfully—may be all that is needed to drive runaway reciprocity
selection. The more different ways that success can be achieved through reciprocity
competition, the more robust will be this type of social selection. This is a kind
of selection that will yield at least part of the human type of social intelligence, perceptiveness,
and perseverance. As we all know, status (or “reputation”) can exist
independent of the adoption of a particular extreme in behavior, so the importance
of any behavioral extreme in changing some aspect of culture can also depend on
whether a prestigious person adopts, favors, or approves of it.
Since Fisherian runaway sexual selection in nonhuman organisms has remained
controversial (or theoretical), but social parallels to it may be robust in human society,
one may wonder if Fisher actually derived his idea from observing human
social situations rather than from thinking about sexual selection in the birds he
ultimately used as his examples. If so, it is likely not the only instance in which an
evolutionist was inspired by human traits and tendencies to develop a general evolutionary
explanation (e.g., Darwin’s observations on human selection of variations
in domestic animals and the fact that Hamilton’s rule applies in its fullest extent
as extensive differential nepotism across multiple levels of relatedness only in humans).
This suggestion is ironic in another way, in view of the success with which
academic biology departments have managed to exclude the human species from
their consideration, leaving its analysis to the almost exclusively nonevolutionary
approaches of social science and medical departments, and surely delaying acceptable
explanations of the human species in evolutionary terms.
When extremes in particular directions are being chosen in social selection,
new extremes never before experienced may be chosen above all other expressions
of a trait. This behavior was originally referred to in European ethology as a
“superoptimal stimulus” effect and is not specifically related to runaway selection.
Whenever superoptimal stimulus effects can be identified, however, they suggest
a history of choosing extremes, therefore the possibility of some form of runaway
selection. If, for example, we perceive that during the past several centuries art—
or even human sociality in general—has flourished and become an enormous and
diverse enterprise compared to any “ancestral” condition it might have exhibited,
we might suspect that this is evidence of a history of choosing extremes continually
in short supply and consequently of some kind of runaway process in however
artistry affects the securing of the resources of reproduction. Rather than evidence
that evolution cannot be used to explain, say, the arts, explanations of recent flowerings
of diversity and complexity in human enterprises can be sought by expanding
our understanding of the various subprocesses of evolutionary selection. We
should not be discouraged because the pathways to explanation become progressively
more difficult and call for expansions of our understanding of the workings
of the evolutionary process."
"Daß man die heutige IQ-Messung eines Tages mit der gleichen überlegenen Rührung betrachten wird, die wir den ersten Dampfmaschinen entgegen bringen, ist allerdings wahrscheinlich."
Dieter E. Zimmer (1975)
[See also: Jensen's critical thoughts on IQ-tests]
Freitag, 25. November 2016
Sonntag, 20. November 2016
Good at school = successful on the job? Explaining gender differences in scholastic and vocational success
Good at school = successful on the job? Explaining gender differences in scholastic and vocational success
Ricarda Steinmayr,
Ursula Kessels
Highlights
- •
- School and work are different environment that require different traits for success.
- •
- GMA, Conscientiousness and Need for Achievement correlate with success in general.
- •
- Agreeableness and Aggression (negatively) correlate with school but not job success.
- •
- Dominance and Affiliation correlate with job but not school success.
- •
- Different personality traits explain gender differences in school and at work.
Abstract
Whereas girls and women outperform men in academic success, men outperform women on vocational success criteria. The present study sought to explain these opposing gender gaps by hypothesizing that, to some extent, different personality traits would promote success in the school and business environments. Using two samples comprised of academic track 11th graders (236) and adult professionals (124), we tested whether gender differences in personality partly explained the opposing gender gaps in academic and vocational success. Questionnaires measuring the Big Five, personality facets, intelligence, and GPA or vocational success criteria were used. Analyses revealed that intelligence, Conscientiousness, and Need for Achievement (AC) predicted both school and vocational success. Agreeableness and Need for Aggression (AG) (negatively) were associated with only academic success. Need for Affiliation (AF) and Need for Dominance (DO) predicted only professional success. Mediation analyses showed that AC, Openness to Experience, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness (girls scored higher), and AG (girls scored lower) mediated gender differences in academic success. Gender differences in vocational success were mediated by DO (men scored higher), whereas AF (women scored higher) suppressed this relation. The results are discussed with respect to their theoretical and practical implications for understanding gender differences in school and at work.
Samstag, 19. November 2016
free online courses:
free online courses (only if you want a certificate there are some costs involved)
https://www.coursera.org/
https://www.coursera.org/
Freitag, 18. November 2016
Donnerstag, 17. November 2016
An Evolutionary Behaviorist Perspective on Orgasm
An Evolutionary Behaviorist Perspective on Orgasm
Diana S. Fleischman (2016)
Abstract
Diana S. Fleischman (2016)
Abstract
Evolutionary explanations for sexual behavior and orgasm most often posit facilitating reproduction as the primary function (i.e. greater rate of fertilization). Other reproductive benefits of sexual pleasure and orgasm such as improved bonding of parents have also been discussed but not thoroughly. Although sex is known to be highly reinforcing, behaviorist principles are rarely invoked alongside evolutionary psychology in order to account for human sexual and social behavior. In this paper, I will argue that intense sexual pleasure, especially orgasm, can be understood as a primary reinforcer shaped by evolution to reinforce behavior that facilitates reproductive success (i.e. conception through copulation). Next, I will describe an evolutionary account of social shaping. In particular, I will focus on how humans evolved to use orgasm and sexual arousal to shape the social behavior and emotional states of others through both classical and operant conditioning and through both reproductive and non-reproductive forms of sexual behavior. Finally, I will describe how orgasm is a signal of sensitivity to reinforcement that is itself reinforcing.
>>>
Orgasm, like other primary reinforcers, can make stimuli that are paired with it (e.g. a color, someone’s face, a specific smell) reinforcing.
Many scientists have been puzzled by women’s orgasmic frequency. Why don’t women consistently experience orgasm and, conversely, why do women have orgasms at all? The fact that women experience orgasm, at least some of the time, is one piece of evidence that orgasm is a primary reinforcer for some kinds of adaptive behavior.
Some (e.g. Prause, 2011) have speculated that it is precisely because orgasm is variable in women that it may be more reinforcing than it is for men; variable reinforcement has been found to be a greater driver of behavior that is resistant to extinction than consistent reinforcement (e.g. gambling on a slot machine is more resistant to extinction than pulling a lever with a consistent payout) (Pryor, 1999).
a woman who was easily orgasmic in a variety of conditions could be making a more costly error: being motivated to engage in behavior that is unlikely to result in the optimal reproductive outcome of conceiving with a genetically fit male who is free of disease (Miller, 2000).
If one considers attachment and affection to another person as a behavior that can be reinforced with orgasm, we might not expect consistent orgasmic and sexual pleasure. With animals, a behavior that is being successfully and consistently produced need not be reinforced consistently (e.g. a dog sitting), but behavior that is decreasing in frequency may need to be carefully shaped when any subtle cue of that behavior reappears. A waning behavior like affection or attachment may be more effectively strengthened by, for example, rewarding the desired cues with variable reinforcement (e.g. the slot machine example used earlier) (Pryor, 1999; Skinner, 1938).
Classical conditioning... is a process by which a neutral stimulus is paired with a primary reinforcer, like sexual pleasure or orgasm, which thereby makes the neutral stimulus in itself reinforcing. When two people engage in sexual behavior and have orgasms they are associating sexual pleasure with the characteristics of the other including proximity, smell, taste, and form; these all become secondary rewards/reinforcers. When two people have repeated erotic contact, they become classically conditioned to perceive one another as secondary reinforcers and can better shape one another toward their own strategic goals.
If one person in a dyad is associated with sexual pleasure, he or she can very easily engage in what is called ‘negative punishment’, withholding positive consequences for a behavior as a means of punishment and an adaptive tactic for extinguishing the behavior. Take, for example, ‘silent treatment’; If an individual values social interaction with a sexual partner, then taking away this reinforcer is a way of simultaneously punishing the behavior and no longer supplying positive reinforcement toward that behavior. Silent treatment will be much more painful if someone’s mere presence, eye contact, or voice is strongly secondarily reinforced by sexual pleasure. Furthermore, withdrawal of other stimuli often associated with orgasm like touch, eye contact, close proximity, and smell can be used more subtly (than silent treatment) to shape behavior both consciously and unconsciously.
A variety of tactics can be used to reinforce and punish others, especially if you are a secondary reinforcer to someone because of classical sexual conditioning. While this hasn’t been explored directly in the literature before, there is one paper that details possible ‘manipulation tactics’ that can be used between romantic partners (Buss, 1989): charm, silent treatment, coercion, reasoning, regression, and debasement. Each of these can be viewed from a social shaping perspective and most could be augmented with the association of sexual pleasure. The ‘charm’ tactic includes acts of love and affection, compliments, gifts, and promising a reciprocal favor in exchange for a behavior in the mate, other means of operantly and classically conditioning a partner than sexual behavior. We already discussed silent treatment above as an example of negative punishment.
Here, I want to stress that positive association between a behavior and a reinforcing outcome like sexual pleasure is unlikely to be overt and, in fact, may work better for strategic goals if the manipulation is covert or unconscious. Self-deception is likely at play here; people are rarely conscious of the ways they manipulate others, and consciously verbalizing how you are manipulating someone else through reinforcement, would likely be viewed as unethical or sociopathic. The tactics of manipulation people use both sexually and non-sexually very likely exceed those they can verbalize.
<<<
Patrick Bateson on “optimal outbreeding”
“Finding a compatible partner is an important part of mate choice. Members of different species do not make good mates. At the other pole, too much inbreeding can also reduce reproductive success. For many years it was supposed that, providing the two extremes were avoided, choice of mate was influenced by all the other factors that are known or assumed to be important … . However, even within the species excessive outbreeding may be costly for a variety of reasons (Bateson, 1980; Shields, 1983). This means that a balance between too much inbreeding and too much outbreeding may have to be struck more carefully than we previously supposed.”
Patrick Bateson (1983)
A Comment on Incest and Marriage Patterns
Source:
Natural Selection and the Analysis of Human Sociality
Richard D. Alexander (1977)
Incest avoidance may be regarded as an aspect of mate selection whereby, in general, close relatives of the opposite sex are unavailable or disfavored, while more distant relatives are favored or specified. Members of one's nuclear family are nearly always excluded as possible mates, and, in humans, certain close relatives, just outside the nuclear family, are nearly always either excluded or disfavored. Other relatives only a little more distant may be suitable, preferred, or prescribed mates. Distant relatives or nonrelatives may also be suitable, preferred, or prescribed mates. If fairly close relatives are preferred or prescribed, those more distant may as a result be more or less incidentally disfavored or excluded.
Genetic outbreeding does not necessarily imply that only the most distant possible relatives, or nonrelatives, are suitable marriage or sex partners. Restricting one's sexual or marriage partner to increasingly distant relatives involves increasing costs (Fig[. ...] 5). Thus, fewer individuals are available, greater distances may have to be traveled to locate them, and greater risks may be involved in securing them; deleterious partitioning of reproductive resources may also result from extreme outbreeding.
On the other hand, close inbreeding also has disadvantages. Thus, with outbreeding fewer deleterious recessive alleles are exposed, there is less chance of reproductive devaluing of relatives because of sexual competition (Hamilton, 1967), and reproductive resources may be advantageously partitioned, for example, to those levels at which they are likely to undergo maximal inflation of their value. The complex patterns of marriage regulations in human societies may thus be largely interpretable as aspects of nepotism in which genetic relatives are constrained against competition, for mates and other resources, which devalues collective effects on the inclusive fitnesses of parents and other relatives. This explanation of marriage patterns and rules focuses attention simultaneously on individual interests in obtaining mates and resources and collective or societal interest in minimizing competition deleterious to the inclusive fitnesses of individuals related to the competitors. Rules or practices resulting from such collective interests must often be compromises based on differences in interests, and differences in power or influence; frequently, the most satisfactory or practical compromise remains the same from one generation to the next. In this case a Darwinian model appears to draw together the phenomena of individual interests and cultural collectivity (including its continuity) in a fashion not previously accomplished with cultural or genetic models that ignored inclusive fitness (Alexander, ms.).
Since the costs of inbreeding diminish outward and the costs of outbreeding mount with its degree, the two lines necessarily cross at some point (Fig. 5). I suggest that in most human societies the lines cross somewhere near the level of first cousins, farther out when more distant cousins are readily available, and closer in when the particular heritable resources involved are deflated rather than inflated in value as a result of extensive partition. Thus, we may expect nomads with few heritable goods, such as Australian Aboriginals, to marry further out. Herdsmen and farmers, and members of titled or royal families, in particular, might tend to marry in. A very large number of factors may be involved in the exact degree of inbreeding and outbreeding, or in the selection of marriage partners at given levels of inbreeding and outbreeding. The important point is that with human marriage patterns, as with kinship systems and inheritance patterns, analyses based on cost-benefit assessments from a Darwinian model are called for, and they are feasible.
The glucocorticoid response in a free-living bird predicts whether long-lasting memories fade or strengthen with time
The glucocorticoid response in a free-living bird predicts whether long-lasting memories fade or strengthen with time (Dec 2016)
Blake Carlton Jones,
Sara E. Bebus
,
Stephen M. Ferguson,
Philip W. Bateman,
Stephan J. Schoech
Highlights
- •
- We threat-conditioned free-living Florida scrub-jays to avoid a novel predator.
- •
- Individuals maintained a conditioned response for 2 years after 5 s of conditioning.
- •
- Corticosterone response positively correlated with memory consolidation.
- •
- Moderate stress responders developed exaggerated defence responses with time.
For decades, scientists have used threat conditioning (traditionally termed ‘fear conditioning’) to study the link between glucocorticoids and the consolidation of long-term memories (i.e. memories that last hours to weeks) in model organisms in the laboratory. We assessed this relationship in a free-living species, and examined a possible relationship between glucocorticoids and the retention of long-lasting memories (i.e. memories that last months to a lifetime). We developed a novel threat-conditioning protocol by which free-living Florida scrub-jays, Aphelocoma coerulescens, were either chased by a novel predator or exposed to a control. We measured flight initiation distance (FID) 48 h, 11 months and 2 years after conditioning or control exposures, and compared these measures to levels of stress-induced glucocorticoids. Conditioned subjects maintained significantly longer FIDs for at least 2 years. Furthermore, the long-term memory consolidation of conditioned subjects positively correlated with their stress-induced glucocorticoid response, similar to results from laboratory studies. Surprisingly, individuals with a moderate stress response exhibited an exaggerated defence response (i.e. FIDs increased) at 11 months and 2 years post-conditioning, whereas low and high stress responders exhibited memory decay or extinction (i.e. FIDs decreased). We speculate that the recently discovered processes of memory reconsolidation and system consolidation may help explain why some Florida scrub-jays exhibit more fearful-like behaviour with time.
Dienstag, 15. November 2016
Some Articles:
Catharine P. Cross, Gillian R. Brown, Thomas J. H. Morgan, Kevin N. Laland (Nov 2016)
Survey of expert opinion on intelligence: The FLynn effect and the future of intelligence
Heiner Rindermann, David Becker, Thomas R. Coyle (Nov 2016)
Heiner Rindermann, David Becker, Thomas R. Coyle (Nov 2016)
Evolutionary framework for identifying sex- and species-specific vulnerabilities in brain development and functions; David C. Geary (Nov 2016)
Paternal age negatively predicts offspring physical attractiveness in two, large, nationally representative datasets; Michael A. Woodley of Menie & Satoshi Kanazawa (Nov 2016)
Ein paar Auszüge aus:
Fühlen, Denken, Handeln
Gerhard Roth (2001)
Fühlen, Denken, Handeln
Gerhard Roth (2001)
“Einem intelligenten Menschen unterstellt man, dass er gut und schnell denken kann, …”
[Demnach] “wäre ein intelligenter Mensch ein solcher, dem angesichts eines bestimmten theoretischen oder praktischen Problems und unter einem gewissen Zeitdruck Lösungen einfallen, auf die Durchschnittsmenschen nicht kommen.”
“Schlechte Problemlöser erkennen oft nicht, was ein Problem schwierig macht, und können ihre Strategien dem neuen Problem nicht gut anpassen. Der intelligente Mensch hingegen ist dadurch gekennzeichnet, dass er relativ schnell eine Lösung des anstehenden Problems findet. Beim Problemlösen muss nicht nur relevante Information aktiviert, sondern auch irrelevante Information unterdrückt werden, und all dies geschieht meist unter Zeitdruck. Hiernach sollte sich das Gehirn eines intelligenten Menschen dadurch auszeichnen, dass das Abrufen relevanter Information (vor allem von »Expertenwissen«), das Unterdrücken irrelevanter Information sowie das anschließende Zusammenfügen der relevanten Information möglichst schnell und mit möglichst geringem Aufwand geschieht.”
“Patienten mit Stirnhirnerkrankungen [sind] oft unfähig, in einer Situation und bei einem Problem Relevantes von Irrelevantem zu unterscheiden. Auch haben sie starke Defizite in der für das Problemlösen wichtigen Fähigkeit, die Perspektive zu wechseln und nach alternativen Strategien zu suchen. Frontalhirn-Patienten verweilen oft hartnäckig bei einer Strategie und lassen sich durch ihr eigenes Versagen nicht beeindrucken.”
“Die meisten von uns leiden an irgendeiner Begrenzung von Gedächtnisleistungen. Wer wünscht sich nicht ein besseres Gedächtnis? Tatsächlich bringt aber ein wirkliches »Supergedächtnis« keineswegs nur Vorteile, denn dies bedeutet keineswegs bloß, dass man sich an alles erinnern kann, wenn man nur will, sondern dass man auch ganz unwichtige Dinge nicht vergessen kann. Ein Supergedächtnis vermag in aller Regel nicht oder nicht gut zwischen »wichtig« und »unwichtig« zu unterscheiden, für es ist nahezu alles »wichtig«, d. h. erinnerungswürdig.”
Dienstag, 8. November 2016
Scott Adams on Passion:
"You often hear advice from successful people that you should “follow your passion.” That sounds perfectly reasonable the first time you hear it. Passion will presumably give you high energy, high resistance to rejection, and high determination. Passionate people are more persuasive, too. Those are all good things, right?
Here’s the counterargument: When I was a commercial loan officer for a large bank in San Francisco, my boss taught us that you should never make a loan to someone who is following his passion. For example, you don’t want to give money to a sports enthusiast who is starting a sports store to pursue his passion for all things sporty. That guy is a bad bet, passion and all. He’s in business for the wrong reason.
My boss, who had been a commercial lender for over thirty years, said the best loan customer is one who has no passion whatsoever, just a desire to work hard at something that looks good on a spreadsheet. Maybe the loan customer wants to start a dry-cleaning store or invest in a fast-food franchise—boring stuff. That’s the person you bet on. You want the grinder, not the guy who loves his job.
So who’s right? Is passion a useful tool for success, or is it just something that makes you irrational?
My hypothesis is that passionate people are more likely to take big risks in the pursuit of unlikely goals, and so you would expect to see more failures and more huge successes among the passionate. Passionate people who fail don’t get a chance to offer their advice to the rest of us. But successful passionate people are writing books and answering interview questions about their secrets for success every day. Naturally those successful people want you to believe that success is a product of their awesomeness, but they also want to retain some humility. You can’t be humble and say, “I succeeded because I am far smarter than the average person.” But you can say your passion was a key to your success, because everyone can be passionate about something or other. Passion sounds more accessible. If you’re dumb, there’s not much you can do about it, but passion is something we think anyone can generate in the right circumstances. Passion feels very democratic. It is the people’s talent, available to all. ..."
Scott Adams (2014)
Sonntag, 6. November 2016
Mittwoch, 2. November 2016
The acquisition of vocabulary:
"People don’t acquire vocabulary by rote learning. They don’t memorize word lists and definitions by drill and repetition. Virtually all of one’s vocabulary is acquired by hearing or reading words in a context from which one can infer their meaning. One might have to encounter a word used in several different contexts to be able to infer its complete meaning and its subtle difference from some similar word (e.g., charitable and generous). Brighter persons infer more of a word’s meaning from any given context and don’t need as many encounters with it in different contexts to grasp its distinctive meaning. Given similar amounts of exposure to the language, therefore, a more intelligent person acquires a larger, qualitatively richer, and more subtle vocabulary than a less intelligent person."
Dienstag, 1. November 2016
Montag, 31. Oktober 2016
Sonntag, 30. Oktober 2016
From Terman to Today: A Century of Findings on Intellectual Precocity
From Terman to Today: A Century of Findings on Intellectual Precocity
David Lubinski (2016)
David Lubinski (2016)
One hundred years of research (1916–2016) on intellectually precocious
youth is reviewed, painting a portrait of an extraordinary source of human
capital and the kinds of learning opportunities needed to facilitate exceptional
accomplishments, life satisfaction, and positive growth. The focus is on those
studies conducted on individuals within the top 1% in general or specific
(mathematical, spatial, or verbal reasoning) abilities. Early insights into the
giftedness phenomenon actually foretold what would be scientifically demonstrated
100 years later. Thus, evidence-based conceptualizations quickly
moved from viewing intellectually precocious individuals as weak and emotionally
liable to highly effective and resilient individuals. Like all groups,
intellectually precocious students and adults have strengths and relative
weaknesses; they also reveal vast differences in their passion for different
pursuits and their drive to achieve. Because they do not possess multipotentiality,
we must take a multidimensional view of their individuality. When done,
it predicts well long-term educational, occupational, and creative outcomes.
Freitag, 28. Oktober 2016
Dienstag, 25. Oktober 2016
100, 200, 400, 800, 1500(men): The athlete's age at breaking the world record:
18: The first 100 m IAAF world record was set by an 18 year old.
19: The first 1500 m IAAF world record was set by a 19 year old.
30: Carl Lewis broke the 100m world record at the age of 30.
31: Michael Johnson broke the 400m world record at the age of 31.
I did not count athletes who equaled a record.
Sources: 100m, 200m, 400m, 800m, 1500;
(All IAAF world records considered until the 25th October 2016.)
Freitag, 21. Oktober 2016
Concept Creep: Psychology's Expanding Concepts of Harm and Pathology
Concept Creep: Psychology's Expanding Concepts of Harm and Pathology
Nich Haslam (2016)
Abstract
Nich Haslam (2016)
Abstract
Many of psychology's concepts have undergone semantic shifts in recent years. These conceptual changes follow a consistent trend. Concepts that refer to the negative aspects of human experience and behavior have expanded their meanings so that they now encompass a much broader range of phenomena than before. This expansion takes “horizontal” and “vertical” forms: concepts extend outward to capture qualitatively new phenomena and downward to capture quantitatively less extreme phenomena. The concepts of abuse, bullying, trauma, mental disorder, addiction, and prejudice are examined to illustrate these historical changes. In each case, the concept's boundary has stretched and its meaning has dilated. A variety of explanations for this pattern of “concept creep” are considered and its implications are explored. I contend that the expansion primarily reflects an ever-increasing sensitivity to harm, reflecting a liberal moral agenda. Its implications are ambivalent, however. Although conceptual change is inevitable and often well motivated, concept creep runs the risk of pathologizing everyday experience and encouraging a sense of virtuous but impotent victimhood.
[via David Schmitt]
[via David Schmitt]
Donnerstag, 20. Oktober 2016
Konrad Lorenz über "Nachsehen" und "Nachdenken":
"Es war eine mehr als kopernikanische Wende in der Entwicklungsgeschichte des menschlichen Erkenntnisstrebens, als die Menschheit zum ersten Mal begann nachzusehen, anstatt wie bisher nachzudenken. Die nach Erkenntnis ihrer selbst und der sie umgebenden Welt ringende Menschheit hat zuerst das Nach-Denken gelernt und erst sehr, sehr viel später das Nach-Sehen! Diese geistesgeschichtliche Tatsache ist deshalb höchst merkwürdig, weil das einfache Nachsehen entwicklungs- und stammesgeschichtlich betrachtet zweifellos eine weit urtümlichere und ältere Leistung darstellt als das Nachdenken."
human diversity:
"Human beings ... are immensely diverse, not only
in their physical characteristics, but even more in their behaviors, personalities,
and capabilities."
John B. Carroll
Montag, 17. Oktober 2016
Samstag, 15. Oktober 2016
Gorillas in our midst? Human sexual dimorphism and contest competition in men
Gorillas in our midst? Human sexual dimorphism and contest competition in men
Alexander K Hill, Drew H Bailey, David A Puts (2016)
Abstract
Alexander K Hill, Drew H Bailey, David A Puts (2016)
Abstract
The literature on human sexual selection has historically focused on the role of female mate choice, but cumulating experimental, correlational, and cross-cultural evidence suggests that male contest competition may have been more influential in shaping men's phenotypes. Cross-species comparison has shown similarities between humans and our closest extant phylogenetic relatives, the Great Apes, in male–male aggression, and archeological evidence also indicates a great antiquity for male–male violence. Compared to women, men possess substantially greater muscle mass, strength, cranial robusticity, physical aggression, pain tolerance, risk-taking, weapons use, and participation in coalitional aggression. Men also exhibit displays of physical prowess and acuity to the formidability of male conspecifics, as well as possessing a suite of traits, such as facial hair and low vocal pitch, that increase perceptions of dominance. These traits are consistent with having been shaped by contest competition over mates: they are sexually dimorphic, appear at sexual maturity, and predict success in male contests as well as success in mating and reproduction. While alternative explanations for some of these sexually dimorphic traits are possible, contest competition among males throughout human evolutionary history is the most parsimonious.
Freitag, 14. Oktober 2016
Tele-Akademie - Prof. Gerhard Roth
Prof. Gerhard Roth spricht zum Thema "Bildung braucht Persönlichkeit" (9.10.2016)
[Naja. Die Aussage Gerhard Roths, dass Gene auf den Ausprägungsgrad der Intelligenz einen untergeordneten Einfluss ausüben, dürfte kaum wissenschaftlich gestützt sein. Meines Erachtens räumt er mit dieser Aussage vorgeburtlichen Umwelteinflüßen eine deutlich zu große Bedeutung ein. In Extremfällen, in denen massiv schlechte Entwicklungsbedingungen im Mutterleib vorhanden sind (bzw. im Fall von extremen Frühgeburten), dürfte dies zu einer erheblichen lebenslangen Veränderung des Intelligenzausprägungsgrad (in negative Richtung) führen. Gerhard Roth schließt aber anscheinend aus schwer nachvollzierbaren Gedankengängen, dass 30 Prozent der Intelligenzvariation aus genetischer Variation, 20 Prozent aus vorgeburtlichen Umwelteinflüssen und die übrige Hälfte aus nachgeburtlichen Umwelteinflüssen (die zum größten Teil in der frühen nachgeburtlichen Phase wirken sollen??) erklärt werden können.]
Self-Esteem and Shame:
"Earlier, I described Shame as the subjective marker of social failure. ... Consistent with this perspective, clinical
evidence indicates that interindividual differences in the ease with which Shame is elicited are
largely the product of differing life histories (Lewis 1987). Significantly, successes and failures
seem to have a cumulative effect -- repeated (and early) failures lower the Shame threshold (Miller
1985). Moreover, both clinical and experimental results indicate that the propensity to experience
Shame is negatively correlated with self-esteem (Retzinger 1991; Miller 1985). Self-esteem can
be conceptualized as the generalized assessment of self as relatively successful or unsuccessful,
a summation of the events which constitute Ego’s self-perceived successes and failures to date. Self-esteem influences whether events are seen as constituting relatively greater or lesser
successes or failures, and it is this which determines the ease of elicitation of Shame. In turn,
because Shame affects the salience of risk and benefit during decision making, by setting the
threshold for experiencing Shame, self-esteem indirectly determines the likelihood that benefit will
be maximized or risk will be avoided. Self-esteem thus operates to create a consistent situation-sensitive
strategy, linking life experience to immediate decisions."
Daniel M. T. Fessler (2001)
"confidence is part of our genetic makeup; it evolved over thousands of generations as a mental tool to guide our decision-making around risk."
Geoffrey Miller
[See also: The role of shame and self-esteem in risk taking by Daniel Fessler, 2001]
Language as a mighty "intelligence amplifier":
>Among terrestrial vertebrates, birds
and mammals generally exceed both amphibians and ‘‘reptiles’’ with respect to
cognitive abilities. Among birds, corvids and parrots stand out in this regard with
behavioral flexibility, innovation rate, tool use and tool fabrication, and also with
respect to truly mental abilities such as logical reasoning and mirror self-recognition––at
least in one corvid species.
Several groups of mammals, like dolphins and whales, dogs, elephants, and
bears (just to mention a few) show signs of high intelligence at least in some
cognitive domains. Primates on average exhibit an intelligence superior to all other
mammals. Among primates, there is a rather clear-cut ranking order in intelligence,
from the prosimians to monkeys and to the great apes. Except a few species
(e.g., capuchins), the great apes exhibit at least some aspects of cognitive and
mental abilities not found in monkeys regarding tool fabrication, insight into
causal mechanisms, mirror recognition, theory of mind, knowledge attribution,
metacognition, and consciousness.
However, humans, even under the most critical aspects, are superior to other
animals in all cognitive functions, no matter how astonishing the achievements of
the latter may be. The most clear-cut differences between humans and non-human
primates lies in two abilities that are interconnected: planning abilities and a
syntactic-grammatical language. When we compare the cognitive-mental abilities
of the most intelligent non-human animals with those of humans, then we find that
they roughly correspond to the abilities of children aged 2 1/2-5. As for linguistic
abilities, chimpanzees and gorillas equal a 3-year-old child, while with respect to
psychosocial abilities (empathy, theory of mind, etc.) they may be equivalent to
those of a 5-year-old child. In light of these empirical findings, the standard
question of whether human intelligence differs qualitatively or only quantitatively from the non-human one, may ironically be transformed into the question about
whether, with respect to cognitive functions, an adolescent or adult is qualitatively
or only quantitatively superior to a 3–5-year-old child. Besides maturation of
social competences, the most decisive feature that distinguishes humans from nonhuman
animals is the appearance of a syntactical-grammatical language at age 2 1/2, which is paralleled by an enormous increase in the capacity of working memory
and, consequently, intelligence, i.e., novel problem solving.
Therefore, the ‘‘rubicon’’ between animal and human intelligence seems to be
the evolution of the syntactical-grammatical language, which is essentially bound
to an increase in the ability to mentally manipulate processes (first actions, then
thoughts, then words) in the temporal domain. Once evolved, human language
served as a mighty ‘‘intelligence amplifier,’’ as was later development of writing
and invention of the computer.<
The long evolution of brains and minds
Gerhard Roth (2013)
Sonderstellung des Menschen:
"Es bleiben ... zwei Dinge, die nach wie vor [in psychischer Hinsicht, den Menschen am deutlichsten von anderen Tieren unterscheiden], nämlich erstens die Fähigkeit, Handlungen mittel- und langfristig zu planen - dies können die nicht-menschlichen Tiere schlecht oder gar nicht - und eben die menschliche, syntaktisch-grammatikalische Sprache."
Gerhard Roth
On the Cognitive and Cultural "Enrichment" of Contemporary Europe by Immigration
On the Cognitive and Cultural "Enrichment" of Contemporary Europe by Immigration
Heiner Rindermann (PFS 2016)
[See also: Discussion, Q & A]
Heiner Rindermann (PFS 2016)
[See also: Discussion, Q & A]
Samstag, 8. Oktober 2016
Freitag, 7. Oktober 2016
Sonntag, 2. Oktober 2016
"Intrinsische Belohnungen bestehen in der Freude am Gelingen, der Selbstbestätigung, im Gefühl der Verwirklichung eigener Fähigkeiten und Wünsche (Selbstwirksamkeit), ..., in der Überzeugung, an einer wichtigen Sache oder in einem >großen Betrieb< mitzuarbeiten. Sie sind die einzige Belohnungsart, die nicht in Sättigung geht und in ihrer Wirkung auch nicht nachlässt, es sei denn aufgrund anhaltender Misserfolge. Oft steigert sich die intrinsische Belohnung mit zunehmender Perfektion einer Tätigkeit und/oder des eintreffenden Erfolgs."
Gerhard Roth
Intuition:
"... Bei vielen komplexen Entscheidungen haben wir das Gefühl, wir sollten uns in einer bestimmten Weise entscheiden, wir können aber keine detaillierten Gründe dafür angeben.
Dies wird oft als >Bauchgefühl< bezeichnet, aber man sollte unter >Bauchgefühlen< eher starke Affekte und Motive verstehen, die uns massiv zu irgendetwas drängen, und solche Zustände treten meist in Situationen auf, die von hohem Stress begleitet sind. Das ist bei Intuitionen anders - sie drängen nicht, sondern legen nahe.
Solche Intuitionen begegnen uns auch beim Bemühen, etwas Kompliziertes zu verstehen, bei künstlerischer oder erfinderischer Tätigkeit. Charakterischerweise zeigen Willensanstrengung und Konzentration meist keine Wirkung, und die >Lösung< ergibt sich in der Regel erst nach einer längeren Pause, in der wir nicht mehr intensiv danach gesucht haben oder völlig abgelenkt sind.
Intuitives Problemlösen unterscheidet sich also qualitativ vom gedanklich konzentrierten Problemlösen dadurch, dass es nicht in klaren rationalen Schritten geschieht, sondern in scheinbar verdeckter Weise, wobei die Lösungsversuche anstrengungslos und ohne Detailerleben verlaufen. Allerdings haben kreative Denker, Erfinder oder Künstler oft das Gefühl, einer Lösung nahe zu sein, sie nur nicht fassen zu können.
Das Langzeitgedächtnis ist der Ort unserer gesamten Erfahrungen und unseres Wissens, sofern diese einmal bewusst waren. Diese Inhalte werden jedoch ... analog zur technischen >Datenkompression< in immer kompakterer Weise gespeichert und auch in dieser kompakten Weise erinnert, wenn sie nicht durch aktives Erinnern wieder >entpackt< werden können. Derart komprimierte Inhalte werden parallel-distributiv verarbeitet und unterliegen deshalb nicht den Beschränkungen des sequenziell arbeitenden Arbeitsgedächtnisses. Deshalb verfügen sie über eine viel höhere Verarbeitungskompetenz, sie brauchen dafür jedoch Zeit, und deshalb sagen wir etwa bei schwierigen Entscheidungen, wir müssten >eine Nacht darüber schlafen<. ..."
Coaching, Beratung und Gehirn
G. Roth & A. Ryba (2016)
Samstag, 1. Oktober 2016
"Der Mensch überragt in kognitiv-geistiger Hinsicht auch
bei kritischer Betrachtung alle anderen Tiere. Aber ist er darin
wirklich einzigartig? Wenn wir die kognitiven Leistungen der
intelligentesten nichtmenschlichen Tiere, der Schimpansen und Gorillas, mit denen des Menschen vergleichen, so kommen wir
zu dem Schluss, dass sie in den meisten kognitiv-geistigen Fähigkeiten dem Entwicklungsstand eines zweieinhalb- bis fünfjährigen
Kindes entsprechen. Hinsichtlich der Sprachfähigkeit
sind Schimpansen und Gorillas eher mit einem dreijährigen
Kind vergleichbar, hinsichtlich psychosozialer Fähigkeiten eher
mit einem vier- bis fünfjährigen Kind. Damit führt die Frage,
ob sich menschliche Intelligenzleistungen quantitativ oder qualitativ
von tierischen unterscheiden, überraschenderweise zu der
Frage, ob ein Jugendlicher oder ein Erwachsener einem drei- bis
fünfjährigen Kind auf kognitiv-geistiger Ebene quantitativ oder
qualitativ überlegen ist. Abgesehen von der Ausreifung sozialer
Kompetenzen geht es hierbei im Wesentlichen um die Entwicklung
der syntaktisch-grammatischen Sprache in einem Alter
von rund zweieinhalb Jahren, die beim Menschen einen großen
intellektuellen und kommunikativen Schub auslöst. Ich habe [zuvor] dargelegt, dass eine
wichtige Bedeutung dieser syntaktisch-grammatikalischen Sprache
in der enormen Steigerung des Arbeitsgedächtnisses besteht
und damit der Fähigkeit, aktuell neuartige Probleme oder
bereits vorhandene Probleme neuartig zu lösen, also in dem,
was man Intelligenz nennt. Die Sprache wird somit zu einem gewaltigen
Intelligenzverstärker, wie es später die Schrift und noch
später der Computer sind."
Wie einzigartig ist der Mensch?
Gerhard Roth (2010)
Cybernetic Approaches to Personality and Social Behavior
Cybernetic Approaches to Personality and Social Behavior
Colin G. DeYoung & Yanna J. Weisberg (2016)
Abstract
Colin G. DeYoung & Yanna J. Weisberg (2016)
Abstract
Cybernetics is the study of principles governing goal-directed, self-regulating, information
processing systems, both artificial and living. Originating in the 1940s and 50s, it helped shape
the cognitive revolution in psychology and has provided crucial insight into cognition and the
control of behavior. This chapter elucidates its basic conceptual features and discusses its
application to understanding personality and social behavior. The central premise is that
cybernetic approaches are promising for the development of solutions to two major problems at
the intersection of personality and social psychology. The first problem is how to describe stable
individual differences in terms of psychological processes that change from moment to moment
in response to situational factors. The second is how to develop explanatory, process-based
theories to accompany the descriptive models of personality, like the Big Five, that have been the
biggest accomplishment of personality psychology in the last half century. Following an
explication of the basic principles of cybernetics, we describe a cybernetic theory of the Big Five
and its applications to theories of social behavior including self-monitoring and interpersonal
theory.
Freitag, 30. September 2016
Sprache als Intelligenzverstärker:
"Was unterscheidet uns ... von Tieren?"
"... Tiere haben keine Sprache wie der Mensch: keine syntaktisch-grammatikalische Sprache. Das ist der Hauptunterschied, eigentlich überhaupt der einzige Unterschied. Nämlich Tiere können auch sehr intelligent sein, ... unsere nächsten Verwandten aber auch Rabenvögel und kleine Affen können sehr intelligent sein. Der Unterschied wenn es um nicht-sprachliches Verhalten geht ist relativ minimal. Sprache ist das was uns als Intelligenzverstärker so außerordentlich klug und intelligent macht. ... Wenn man nämlich Kinder und Erwachsene für eine gewisse Zeit daran hindert zu sich selber zu sprechen während sie denken, dann fallen sie praktisch auf die Intelligenzebene von Schimpansen zurück. ... Also Sprache ist ein außerordentlicher Intelligenzverstärker."
Abonnieren
Posts (Atom)