Bo Winegard et al.:
"To clarify the logic of the coalitional value theory, we recapitulate it briefly below.
1) Humans are motivated to strive for status because status
offers better access to resources that enhance fitness such
as mates, food, and prestige goods. Copious research has
found that humans are motivated to obtain status
(Anderson et al. 2015; Barkow 1975; Von Rueden et al.
2011; Vonasch et al. 2018). Status allows people priority
access to coveted resources such as shelter, food, mates,
and prestige goods that enhance genetic fitness.
Therefore, this motivation is likely intrinsic and arises
across all cultures.
2) People freely defer to socially valuable partners and
coalitional members who have high value. For much of
evolutionary history, status was determined by coercive
threats (dominance). However, at some point in the hominid lineage, a new kind of status dynamic evolved, one in
which humans freely deferred to others with high
coalitional value (as social partners) (Henrich and GilWhite 2001), because these others disproportionately
contributed to the wellbeing of other group members.
Deference may serve the function of keeping these individuals in the group and providing them with more control to make often group-enhancing decisions; these of
course would also be self-enhancing. Physical intimidation can certainly evoke deference from others, but physical intimidation will not get the best output from others,
especially if those others are skilled in some important
way. Social deference then confers status for producing
outputs that benefit the deferent, without resorting to
intimidation.
3) People expect others to defer to coalitional members with
higher coalitional value. Not only do people defer to social partners with high coalitional value, but also they
expect other group members to do so. If other group
members do not do so, they are often ostracized or
punished. Those who punish the recalcitrant group members are recompensed for their efforts with status because
promoting cooperation increases one’s own coalitional
value.
4) People defer in these predictable ways because they have
a coalitional value gauge. To defer to people with high
coalitional value, humans must have a mental mechanism
that assesses coalitional value. The mechanism must interface with other mental systems that lead either to deference or assertion (or indifference). They also have a
group status gauge that assess the overall fitness prospects
of groups/coalitions. (This gauge is probably a part of a
mental mechanism that first distinguishes ingroup from
outgroup.)
5) Coalitional value is not often immediately obvious, so the
system must use cues, especially from the visual and auditory systems. Like other important traits, coalitional value is not directly perceivable. Therefore, people must use
cues and signals to assess another person’s coalitional
value. Such cues might include confident facial expressions, displays of skill, upper body strength, assertive
voice, physical size, erect posture, etc. (Holbrook and
Fessler 2013; Sell et al. 2012; Lukaszewski et al. 2016)
For example, monarchs throughout most of history wore
gaudy and extravagant clothes and diadems made of precious metals. These regalia signaled to others that the
monarch was powerful and possessed high value to the
coalition.
6) Because people are motivated to obtain status, they are
often motivated to increase their competence (which is
basically an estimation of their potential coalitional
value). According to many researchers and studies,
humans are intrinsically motivated to increase their competence (Ryan and Deci 2017). An increase in competence is generally associated with at least a potential increase in coalitional value (e.g., if one becomes a more
competent tactician, then one increases one’s value to a
military unit); and an increase in coalitional value is generally associated with a potential increase in status that in
turn increases social influence access to valued resources.
7) Increases in coalitional value heighten self-esteem and
decreases dampen it. Numerous researchers have drawn
attention to the association between self-esteem and social value (Leary 2005; Leary et al. 1995; Mahadevan
et al. 2016). However, there is no consensus about precisely what the relation is. Some have contended that it is
between self-esteem and relational value (e.g., Leary
2005); others have argued that it is between self-esteem
and status (e.g., Mahadevan et al. 2018). Our suggestion
is that self-esteem might also track coalitional value such
that increases in coalitional value temporarily heighten
self-esteem and decreases temporarily reduce it.
8) Hierarchies are more or less stable depending upon three
factors: Overall group status, severity of group competition, and the degree to which the hierarchy is arranged
based on individuals’ coalitional value. Other things
equal, the more severe group competition is, the more
people commit to a coalition, which strengthens the stability of the hierarchy. The same is true of group status.
The higher a group’s status vis-à-vis other groups, the
more stable the hierarchy will be. Of course, the elites
will still vie for control of crucial resources and status.
And, last, the more the arrangement of the hierarchy is
based on coalitional value rather than other features of the
individuals that comprise it (e.g., family relations), the
more stable the hierarchy will be, because subordinates
receive the maximal resources that are possible for them
to have given the skills they have to offer. If the people on
top of the social hierarchy have lower coalitional value
than those at the bottom, more people in the group will
suffer than is necessary."
https://gwern.net/doc/genetics/selection/natural/human/2020-winegard.pdf
Keine Kommentare:
Kommentar veröffentlichen