Donnerstag, 2. Oktober 2025

Deference:

Bo Winegard et al.:

"Who defers to whom? Who listens to whom? The solution to this coordination problem, we believe, is the coalitional value system. The person who most benefits the group, the best leader or warrior, receives deference from others."

Reliability:

"In the occupational world, reliability is one of the foundations of trust and teamwork.

  • If colleagues can rely on you, projects run smoother and stress is reduced.

  • Reliability means deadlines are met, commitments are honored, and communication is clear.

  • It builds your professional reputationpeople see you as dependable.

  • Unreliable colleagues, on the other hand, create frustration, extra work, and damaged trust.

  • In many jobs, reliability isn’t just about productivity but also about safety and responsibility.

In short: being reliable is not optional — it’s essential if you want to be respected and effective at work."

When Work Becomes Identity:

Some Notes:

Why do some people seem so strongly intertwined with their profession, in a way that the profession can hardly be separated from them?

With other people, such a fit or interweaving seems hardly to exist.

I recall a remark by @datepsych that many people tend to define themselves much more by their beliefs or assumptions, rather than by what they actually do. Yet, in the end, it is what one does that really counts. After all, adopting or presenting a particular belief can be done with relatively little effort. But to truly be able to do something, to acquire some deeper skills, always requires effort.

I also remember Colin DeYoung’s distinction between characteristic adaptations and personality traits. For example, if I practice a profession over several years, I acquire new and potentially important characteristic adaptations, even though my personality traits hardly change in the process.

And finally, general learning ability probably plays a role in how much someone can become intertwined with his profession—that is, how much knowledge and skill a person can acquire in a given field.

Aircraft Carriers:

Only countries in the Northern Hemisphere operate aircraft carriers.

Source: Wikipedia
  Countries currently operating fixed-wing aircraft carriers (10)   Countries currently solely operating helicopter carriers (6)   Historic operators of carriers (3)


Tension versus Comfort:

"But I don't want comfort. I want God, I want poetry, I want real danger, I want freedom, I want goodness. I want sin."

Aldous Huxley, Brave New World


In a world of action, tension is everywhere — and those who crave comfort often retreat from action.

The Perils of Audience Capture:

Gurwinder shows that social media audiences have pushed some influencers’ behavior and personality in strange directions.

https://www.gurwinder.blog/p/the-perils-of-audience-capture

Mittwoch, 1. Oktober 2025

Coalitional Value Theory:

Bo Winegard et al. (2020):

"humans evolved unique mental mechanisms for assessing each other’s marginal value to a coalition (i.e., each other’s coalitional value). They defer to those with higher coalitional value, and they assert themselves over those with lower."

-----

"The primary construct of the CVT is a mental gauge that estimates and tracks one’s own and others’ values to the coalition. For simplicity, we will call this a coalitional value gauge (gauge for evaluating self and for evaluating others). The gauge that evaluates the self’s coalitional value is likely strongly related to what social psychologists have traditionally called self-esteem (e.g., Mahadevan et al. 2018). The information from the gauge is fed into a number of other mental systems, causing a variety of physiological, behavioral, and cognitive responses. For example, if the gauge calculated that another person in one’s coalition has higher value than one’s self, then the gauge, through interactions with other systems, would produce predictable emotional and cognitive responses such as awe, admiration, deference, increased blood pressure, reverence, visual attention, etc. (Keltner and Haidt 2003; Long et al. 1982). Of course, these aspirational and positive emotions might be tinged with envy and bitterness, especially if the high-status person is rude or dismissive (Buss 2001). If, on the other hand, the gauge calculated that another person was lower, then it would produce assertion, contempt, erect posture, expected subordination, etc. These responses are likely heightened in coalitionally relevant contexts."

-----

"People defer in these predictable ways because they have a coalitional value gauge. To defer to people with high coalitional value, humans must have a mental mechanism that assesses coalitional value. The mechanism must interface with other mental systems that lead either to deference or assertion (or indifference)."

-----

"Specifically, according to our approach, conceptions of god are designed to appeal to people’s propensity to defer to others who are high in coalitional value (see, for example, Kirkpatrick 2001). God (or gods) is often depicted as having really high coalitional value (this changes across time, as we will note). And by providing believers with a common “leader” to whom they can defer, conceptions of god potentially create a more cohesive, competitive coalition. Mortals, all of whom accept that god has the highest coalitional value of the group, submit to god, but also compete to be seen as especially approved and close to him or her, increasing their own coalitional value and legitimacy.

-----

Source (a short review will follow):
Coalitional Value Theory: an Evolutionary Approach to Understanding Culture
Bo Winegard et al. (2020)
https://gwern.net/doc/genetics/selection/natural/human/2020-winegard.pdf

[See also: Social Selection / Partner Selection and Partner Rejection]

-----

"more successful groups allocated status more wisely (i.e., for behaviors that helped the group), therefore perpetuating the group and the group’s norms."

-----

"A charismatic leader can benefit a group by making it more coherent and cooperative (using narratives and charm to create group bonds); in exchange, the members of the group defer to the charismatic leader, allowing him or her priority access to coveted resources."

-----

"status (deference) is exchanged for something (e.g., leadership, proximity, immediate resources)"

-----

It is worth noting that coalitional value and status are not the same thing. Coalitional value is the marginal fitness value a person adds to a coalition; status is the amount of power via deference he or she wields over subordinates.

-----

"Researchers generally divide status into prestige: status in which subordinates defer because they desire rewards from the prestigious person, and dominance: status in which subordinates defer because they fear punishment from the dominant person."

-----

"Although coalitional value often leads to status, a person’s coalitional value and his or her status can be discrepant. In general, when a person strives for status that he or she does not deserve (i.e., status that is higher than his or her coalitional value would predict), then that person will have to resort to tactics of dominance such as coercion, insults, threat of force, etc."

-----

Prestige is freely conferred because the person’s coalitional value is high and therefore they provide commensurate rewards to a coalition; dominance, on the other hand, is not freely conferred because the dominant individual is usually trying to garner more status than his coalitional value would suggest he should have.

-----

"We assume that coalitional value mechanisms ... evolved over many millions of years, and may be present to some degree in chimpanzees."

-----

"many animals defer to other, more dominant animals to avoid costly and potentially deadly agonistic encounters (Maynard Smith and Price 1973). In some sense, even a crude dominance/pecking order follows the logic of a status exchange system. The beta animal defers to avoid death and the alpha accepts to avoid potentially costly injuries and to secure the benefits of helpful subordinates in future coalitional conflicts, which increase the alpha’s reproductive success"

-----

"One important challenge a large group faces is coordination: Who defers to whom? Who listens to whom? The solution to this coordination problem, we believe, is the coalitional value system. The person who most benefits the group, the best leader or warrior, receives deference from others. But, not only does he receive deference but also people believe that other group members should defer to him. They regulate other people in the coalition, urging them to defer to the leader, because if the leader has maximum deference (and therefore maximum resources) the leader will best be able to benefit the group as a whole. This likely led, after many generations, to the psychological capacities and propensities that make human social life so unique in the animal kingdom: “we intentionality,” exquisite theory of mind, complex groups with specialized roles, social norms, and on and on (Smaldino 2020; Wellman et al. 2001)."

-----

"The primary construct of the CVT is a mental gauge that estimates and tracks one’s own and others’ values to the coalition. For simplicity, we will call this a coalitional value gauge (gauge for evaluating self and for evaluating others). The gauge that evaluates the self’s coalitional value is likely strongly related to what social psychologists have traditionally called self-esteem (e.g., Mahadevan et al. 2018). The information from the gauge is fed into a number of other mental systems, causing a variety of physiological, behavioral, and cognitive responses. For example, if the gauge calculated that another person in one’s coalition has higher value than one’s self, then the gauge, through interactions with other systems, would produce predictable emotional and cognitive responses such as awe, admiration, deference, increased blood pressure, reverence, visual attention, etc. (Keltner and Haidt 2003; Long et al. 1982). Of course, these aspirational and positive emotions might be tinged with envy and bitterness, especially if the high-status person is rude or dismissive (Buss 2001). If, on the other hand, the gauge calculated that another person was lower, then it would produce assertion, contempt, erect posture, expected subordination, etc. These responses are likely heightened in coalitionally relevant contexts."

-----

"Very few people want to belong to groups that are destined for persistent failure and incredibly low status even if doing so would guarantee high (relative) coalitional value, e.g., most people would not choose to be the best engineer for a destitute company."

-----


Is it impossible to build general artificial intelligence?

A short essay on a hot topic by Emil Kirkegaard.

https://emilkirkegaard.dk/en/2025/08/is-it-impossible-to-build-general-artificial-intelligence/

Freeloaders, Bossybritches, and Control Freaks:

An entertaining essay by edrealist:
(hbd in action)

https://educationrealist.wordpress.com/2025/05/11/freeloaders-bossybritches-and-control-freaks/

Flex Appeal:

Flex Appeal: The Relationship between Exercise Frequency and Mate Value

Kyle E. Conlon & Lauren E. Brewer (2025)


Abstract

Exercise frequency may serve as a proximal cue of one’s potential mate value; however, surprisingly few studies have investigated the explicit link between these constructs. Drawing on evolutionary theories, Dobersek et al. (Dobersek et al., Evolutionary Psychological Science 7:124–133, 2021) found that more frequent exercisers scored higher in self-perceived mate value than less frequent exercisers. In the present study, we replicated and extended Dobersek et al.’s findings by investigating the relationships among exercise frequency, mate value, intrasexual competition, and mate retention. Participants (N = 683) reported their exercise frequency and completed measures of self-perceived mate value and ideal partner mate value. Participants also completed measures of intrasexual competition and mate retention concern. Consistent with Dobersek et al., self-perceived mate value was higher among individuals who exercised more rather than less frequently. Extending their findings, high-frequency exercisers also reported a higher ideal partner mate value, and a significant sex × exercise frequency interaction was observed in which males who exercised less frequently reported the lowest ideal partner mate value. Finally, we found partial support for the predictions that high-frequency exercisers would derive greater satisfaction from outcompeting intrasexual rivals and that low-frequency exercisers would show greater concern over the potential dissolution of their relationships. Implications for mate selection and mating success are discussed.

The Bimodal Age Distribution of Mass Murder

The Bimodal Age Distribution of Mass Murder: a Systematic Review Using Evolutionary and Life History Perspectives (2025)

Keith Minihane, Maria Dempsey & Robert King



Abstract

Research has identified a bimodal age distribution among mass murderers, with younger offenders typically in late adolescence and older offenders in middle age. An evolutionary perspective offers a complementary framework for interpreting differences in motivations, stressors, and target selections across offender subtypes. Drawing from Life History Theory (Stearns, 1976) and evolutionary models of male competition, and resource control (Daly & Wilson, 1988; Duntley & Buss, 2011), this study explores how age-specific environmental stressors interact with evolved psychological mechanisms, shaping pathways to mass violence. A systematic review was conducted in April 2022, using Web of Science, Scopus, PsycARTICLES, PsycINFO, and a manual search. Of 634 studies retrieved, 20 peer-reviewed empirical studies met the stringent inclusion criteria, specifically examining the triggers, and stressors, experienced by mass murderers. A search in September 2024 identified no additional studies. Eligible studies were quality-assessed, and narratively synthesized to identify recurring psychological patterns, and situational triggers. Findings suggest that younger offenders often experience chronic rejection, and status deprivation, leading to public acts of retaliatory violence as an assertion of dominance, and a means of achieving notoriety. In contrast, older offenders, facing acute status losses such as divorce or financial collapse, were more likely to engage in familicide and suicidality, reflecting a maladaptive “last resort” strategy. Evolutionary models of male competition and risk-taking offer insight into why early-life social rejection can increase status-driven aggression, while midlife crises may provoke desperate attempts to regain control over one’s reproductive legacy. By integrating proximate stressors with evolutionary and life history frameworks, this review supplements existing criminological and psychological perspectives, providing an additional interpretive lens for understanding offender subtypes. While evolutionary theory does not offer an alternative model of mass violence, it is an added value perspective which highlights consistent patterns that may inform future research, risk assessment, and prevention strategies.

Relational Aggression and Lifetime Offspring:

Relational Aggression and Lifetime Offspring: A Preliminary Study in a Large Community-Based Sample of Polish Adults (2025)


Marcin Moroń (nice name!)


Abstract

Relational aggression is postulated as an alternative evolutionary strategy by which an individual may achieve social position as well as access to mates and resources in a covert manner, and avoid costs of overt confrontation. However, previous studies rarely focused on present-day natural selection for the disposition to be relationally aggressive in terms of ultimate indices of reproductive success (e.g., lifetime number of children). The present study addressed this limitation by studying links between peer-directed and romantic partner-directed relational aggression, and lifetime (age-controlled) offspring in a general Polish population (N = 1,497; Mage = 47.84; SDage = 15.73; 52.30% women). We used correlation analysis and fitted Poisson regression models. People high on peer-directed relational aggression have been involved in romantic relationships more frequently. After controlling for age, being involved in a romantic relationship and socioeconomic status, a higher disposition to be relationally aggressive toward one’s peers was associated with higher fertility. Among the romantically involved participants, romantic partner-directed relational aggression was also positively associated with fertility. The results indicated that relational aggression might be under weak, yet positive directional selection which has implications for understanding this form of aggressive behavior in present-day humans.